A new study finds that, under the right conditions, 2
1/2-year-old children can answer questions about people acting on false
beliefs, an ability that most researchers believe does not develop until
age 4.
The results are reported in the Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
“Having the ability to represent false beliefs means recognizing that
others can have different thoughts from us,” said Peipei Setoh, who, as
a graduate student, conducted the study with University of Illinois
psychology professor Renée Baillargeon and fellow graduate student Rose
Scott. Setoh is now a professor at Nanyang Technological University,
Singapore.
Young children’s understanding of others’ false beliefs is at the
heart of a debate among psychologists trying to explain years of
inconsistent findings. Some think that false-belief understanding
develops at age 4, when children can answer direct questions about it.
Others, including Baillargeon, believe the tests psychologists
traditionally use are too difficult for young children to successfully
demonstrate their understanding of how others view the world.
“The field is very divided and we are trying to reconcile all of the evidence,” Baillargeon said.
In the study, she and her colleagues found that 30- and 33-month-old
toddlers were able to successfully demonstrate false-belief
understanding using a modified version of a well-known test.
Called the Sally-Anne test, the experiment evaluates a child’s
expectations of how someone will act based on that person’s false
beliefs. If Sally hides a toy in a basket before she leaves the room,
when she returns she expects the toy to be where she left it, in the
basket. If her friend Anne moves the toy from the basket to a box while
Sally is away, Sally will still think the toy is in the basket when she
returns. Someone observing this scene will expect Sally to act on that
false belief.
When directly asked about the expected location in this test,
4-year-old children are able to correctly identify the basket. However,
younger children respond with the actual, and not the expected, location
of the toy.
Baillargeon’s previous research shows that children’s nonverbal
behavior – for example, looking longer when something unexpected happens
– confirms that they understand others’ false beliefs, even though the
children are unable to verbally convey it in the traditional test.
She hypothesizes that toddlers fail at traditional false-belief
understanding tests because the test design overwhelms their ability to
pay attention and respond appropriately. To address this flaw, she
developed a simpler version of the Sally-Anne test, in which she uses a
character named Emma. In the traditional task, when asked the direct
question about where Sally will look for her toy, young children
struggle to suppress information they have about the actual versus
expected location. In Baillargeon’s new version of the test, Emma’s toy
is moved to an unknown location completely out of the scene.
“It’s much easier to inhibit or suppress that response when you don’t know where the toy actually is,” Baillargeon said.
The new approach also gives children a chance to prepare for the test question by giving them two practice questions.
With these modifications to the test, both 30- and 33-month-old
toddlers are able to tell researchers where Emma will look for the
object, verbally demonstrating their understanding that Emma has a false
belief about the object’s location.
Baillargeon and her colleagues found that 33-month-old toddlers,
but not 30-month-olds, were able to answer correctly when given two
practice trials that were different from the test trial. Receiving only
one practice trial foiled all of the toddlers however, as did having the
object moved to another hiding location in the scene and not taken
away.
“What we are showing is that a little bit of practice goes a long
way,” Baillargeon said. Other researchers have reported that children
with more exposure to talking about other’s mental states – including
children with more siblings, or better language ability – perform better
on the traditional Sally-Anne task.
“Practice will increase the range of tasks where you can show
false-belief understanding, because you won’t be taken so much by
surprise with the question,” she said.
With her ongoing research, Baillargeon hopes to identify the
necessary conditions for young children to succeed at more difficult
versions of false-belief tests.
I am a professional ICT personnel, Chief System Analyst, blogger, Managing Director/Chief Executive Officer at Gatmond Internationals inc. and Country Director at Wake Up For Your Right Internationals USA (Nigeria Branch).
Subscribe to:
Post Comments (Atom)
AUTOMATIC RANKING OF TERTIARY INSTITUTIONS IN DEVELOPING NATIONS (A CASE STUDY OF NIGERIA UNIVERSITIES)
University ranking has become increasingly important in recent years among the general public at large because it creates a public platform...
-
SEO & Google AdWords Certified Freelance Consultant in Connecticut 860.633.6578 Professional SEO Services , Google AdWords S...
-
Surprising Facts About Vitamin H Vitamin H comes from the B-complex group of Vitamins. Vitamin H cannot be synthesized by the human b...
No comments:
Post a Comment